Which Canine Curriculum Should You Choose?
What science has to say about the best way to train your dog
Lynne Peeples • November 12, 2009
Paybacks of positive reinforcement
Before practicing professionally as a dog trainer, Jolanta Benal of Brooklyn, New York, learned the difference between positive and punitive methods personally.
Her dog, Muggsy, had an attraction to men in uniform. Whether they were wearing UPS brown or U.S. Postal Service blue, Benal’s pit bull mix would lunge at them on the street. So she hired a highly recommended dog trainer to try to correct this behavior.
Learning by Reward “Always set them up to succeed,” says JoAnne Basinger, as she lines up a series of turquoise cones, spaced about five feet apart, on the rubberized black floor. Four dogs and their owners, each sporting what looks like climber’s chalk bags, are participating in an obedience course at Andrea Arden’s Dog Training in New York City’s SoHo neighborhood. At each cone, the owners are instructed to reward their dogs for staying at their “third shoe” with a small treat from their pouches. The room echoes of clicks from a device positive trainers use to quickly inform a dog that they have done something right, before providing the tasty reinforcement. The dogs continue marching up and down the line of cones, most eyes locked on their owners in anticipation. The distraction of other dogs walking within a few feet of him, however, turns out to be too much for a black-and-white Pomeranian puppy named Casper. He lunges at a classmate. “When he gets kooky like that and loses focus, just let him hold still for a while,” instructs Basinger. Then, when the dog regains focus on his owner, she suggests giving another treat. “But the reward should always disappear when the behavior you want goes away.” The only contact the owners make with their dogs during Basinger’s class is to provide praise. Basinger tells the class to always help the dog associate hand contact solely with positive reinforcement, not punitive punishment. “You want them to know it’s always a good thing,” she says. “Otherwise it’s a time bomb that could go off, possibly when children are around.” The same goes for indirect contact down the chain: no dog in attendance wore a choke collar. “It shouldn’t be the leash that teaches them where to be,” Basinger says. “It’s the information you give.” |
“He would set Muggsy up to do offending behavior, and then throw a can full of pennies at the dog,” she says. “It was a traditional old school technique. And it worked to suppress the problem behavior — at least in the moment.” Muggsy’s unhealthy obsession with the postal workers, however, did not go away. Even if he didn’t always jump at the UPS guy on a walk-by, says Benal, he wasn’t happy to see him either.
Benal then traded in for a new trainer that brought chicken instead of coins. As the man in uniform approached, Benal was now instructed to distract Muggsy by giving him the treat. And it worked. After several times, the dog would look to her in expectation, rather than towards the uniform-clad men in alarm. “For the last year of his life, he was an angel,” says Benal. “It was amazing the changes it brought.”
Millan argues that using food to coax dogs may be impractical: “It can result in an addiction to treats or an overweight dog,” he says in an email. However, Dodman of Tufts University explains that trainers only give food at the beginning of training. After a period of time, owners should reward intermittently, reinforcing the response. “If every time you played the lottery you won money, then the excitement wouldn’t be there anymore,” says Dodman. “The thrill for the dog is ‘Will I get a treat this time?’” Back-aches from stooping low to feed a dog, or the added cost of extra chicken or doggy treats, he believes, are far less dreadful than the anxiety and altered relationships caused by the punitive alternative.
Dodman has some data to back him up. In February 2004, a paper in Animal Welfare by Elly Hiby and colleagues at the University of Bristol compared the relative effectiveness of the positive and punitive methods for the first time. The dogs became more obedient the more they were trained using rewards. When they were punished, on the other hand, the only significant change was a corresponding rise in the number of bad behaviors.
A series of more recent papers also support Dodman’s theory and Hiby’s results. A study published in the October 2008 issue of Journal of Veterinary Behavior found that positive reinforcement led to the lowest average scores for fear and attention-seeking behaviors, while aggression scores were higher in dogs of owners who used punishment. Another 2008 study, this one published inApplied Animal Behavior Science, found that positive training methods resulted in better performances than punishment for Belgian military dog handlers.
Bridging the differences in dogma
It’s hard to argue that the slow, patient techniques used in positive reinforcement would elicit the same dramatic moments seen on Cesar Millan’s show. “There’s a big difference between looking at behavior as a ‘Stop that’ versus a ‘Here’s what I want,’” says Bruce Blumberg, a professor of dog psychology at the Harvard Extension School. “Positive reinforcement is a different mindset. And it’s one that doesn’t work quite as well on TV.”
Dodman is one of many people who have asked the National Geographic Channel to discontinue “The Dog Whisperer,” consistently one of the highest-rated shows on the network. The American Humane Association issued a press statement in 2006 asking for a cancellation because of what they suggested were abusive techniques used by Millan. More recently, the American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior issued a position statement in which it expresses concern “with the recent reemergence of dominance theory and forcing dogs and other animals into submission as a means of preventing and correcting behaviors.”
Millan defends his methods, asserting they “use the minimum force necessary to prevent or correct a problem.” According to the dog rehabilitator, he can “redirect the behavior of most of my pack with just my body language, eye contact and energy.” He points to the “thousands upon thousands of letters” he receives from viewers touting “miracles” of restored relationships and saved dogs. “All I want is what is best for the animal,” Millan says.
Despite the controversy, there is a lot that everyone agrees on. Both sides of the training spectrum teach that a lack of discipline or structure is not conducive to a well-behaved dog. “Dogs need direction and boundaries, just like human relationships,” says Haggerty, the trainer with her own dog school in Manhattan, which uses dominance theory. “If dogs don’t know what the boundaries are, they will wreak havoc.”
How a dog owner projects those boundaries is also important. “You have to be calm, you have to be clear, you have to be consistent, and you have to make sure you meet your pet’s needs for other things: exercise, play, social interaction,” says Herron of The Ohio State University.
So what does an owner do when a calm and structured environment still breeds a misfit pup like JonBee? Should it be the leash and hand that redirects the dog, or poultry and patience? Current science favors the chicken flavor. But whichever strategy you choose, everyone agrees that the timing must be precise. It is very difficult for a dog to make an appropriate association and learn from the reprimand or reward otherwise.
Of course, if you take Blumberg’s Harvard class, he’ll tell you, “If your timing is lousy using positive reinforcement, the worst thing that happens is you get a fat dog.”
3 Comments
Interesting article. I’ve spent years working with dogs at a veterinary hospital and while I have always been in awe of Cesar’s apparent transformation of dogs its a fine line between a submissive dog and a fearful dog. Scared animals are very unpridictable and while they may seem behaved they can easily snap if provoked. On the other hand dogs will do almost anything for food, motivating them in this way is always prefered. There are instances where dogs are not motivated by food or affection and are fearful by nature these situations are much more difficult. I think that both training methods are equally valid if used on a case by case basis. One approach will not work for every pet. Certainly a reward based system should always be tried first.
“Dominance Theory” as stated, does sound like you just beat the dog into doing whatever you want. That however, is not the case. You don’t snap a prong collar on a puppy just learning to sit, but at the same time, throwing a chicken leg at a dog who is attacking someone doesn’t help either. The level of correction has to be appropriate to the disobedience. When working with an actual qualified trainer, a “Dismissed” command is always given at the end of training, it is a stress reliever, a cue to let the dog know that it has done well and it can relax. The dogs are still able to be played with, but they understand they have to work for it first. In the beginning, it seems like more work then play, but as training goes on, they balance out and your dog has much more opportunities available to them.
i think the dominance theory is the best way forward, also Cesar Millan has such a way with animals, all credit to him, this was a really good article thanks for the read!